KirtC.Butler;LarryH.P.Lang
JournalofAccountingResearch,Vol.29,No.1.(Spring,1991),pp.150-156.
StableURL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0021-8456%28199121%2929%3A1%3C150%3ATFAOIA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-RJournalofAccountingResearchiscurrentlypublishedbyTheInstituteofProfessionalAccounting,GraduateSchoolofBusiness,UniversityofChicago.
YouruseoftheJSTORarchiveindicatesyouracceptanceofJSTOR'sTermsandConditionsofUse,availableat
http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html.JSTOR'sTermsandConditionsofUseprovides,inpart,thatunlessyouhaveobtainedpriorpermission,youmaynotdownloadanentireissueofajournalormultiplecopiesofarticles,andyoumayusecontentintheJSTORarchiveonlyforyourpersonal,non-commercialuse.
Pleasecontactthepublisherregardinganyfurtheruseofthiswork.Publishercontactinformationmaybeobtainedathttp://www.jstor.org/journals/grad-uchicago.html.
EachcopyofanypartofaJSTORtransmissionmustcontainthesamecopyrightnoticethatappearsonthescreenorprintedpageofsuchtransmission.
TheJSTORArchiveisatrusteddigitalrepositoryprovidingforlong-termpreservationandaccesstoleadingacademic
journalsandscholarlyliteraturefromaroundtheworld.TheArchiveissupportedbylibraries,scholarlysocieties,publishers,andfoundations.ItisaninitiativeofJSTOR,anot-for-profitorganizationwithamissiontohelpthescholarlycommunitytakeadvantageofadvancesintechnology.FormoreinformationregardingJSTOR,pleasecontactsupport@jstor.org.
http://www.jstor.orgFriOct1911:54:092007
Journal of Accounting Research Vol. 29 No. 1Spring 1991 Printed in U.S.A. Capsules and Comments The Forecast Accuracy of Individual Analysts: Evidence of Systematic Optimism and Pessimism KIRT C. BUTLER* AND LARRY H. P. LANGt 1. Introduction O'Brien [I9901 examines the annual earnings forecast accuracy of individual analysts reporting to the Institutional Brokers Estimate System (IBES)of Lynch, Jones and Ryan during July 1975-September 1982. Using a regression model which adjusts for average firm and year effects, O'Brien finds no evidence of differential forecast accuracy. Comparing individual analysts' earnings forecasts to median consensus forecasts during 1983-86, we also find no statistically significant evidence of differential analyst forecast accuracy. Our result, however, does not mean that one analyst's forecasts are much like another's. We find that analysts are persistently optimistic or pessimistic relative to consensus forecasts. We examine this persistent behavior and its effect on measures of forecast accuracy over a sample * Michigan State University; t New York University. We gratefully acknowledge the comments of Warren Bailey, Peter Bernstein, K. C. Chan, David Girard, Dorian Feldman, David Mayers, Patricia O'Brien, and John Schreiber on previous drafts of this manuscript and the assistance of Dale Berman and Karen Waldemar of Lynch, Jones and Ryan. Funding was provided by Bell Atlantic Corporation and Michigan State University. 150 Copyright 0,Institute of Professional Accounting 1991 FORECAST ACCURACY OF INDIVIDUAL ANALYSTS 151 period in which analyst forecasts consistently overestimate actual earnings. Our sample consists of forecasts of annual earnings for 49 firms with December year-ends selected from the top 65 firms in the December 1986 Fortune index. Firms undergoing stock splits, stock dividends, or mergers are omitted. Individual analysts7 forecasts of current-year primary earn- ings per share before extraordinary items (IBES field \"FYI\") and the analysts7 self-reported forecast dates were provided by Lynch, Jones and Ryan for 30 of the 48 months during 1983-86. Forecasts reported to IBES on a diluted basis are changed to primary EPS according to the ratio of fully diluted EPS to primary EPS in Compustat. Compustat is also the source of earnings and share price data. Empirical results focus on the 9,120 forecasts of 186 analysts providing at least three earnings estimates in each sample year. 2. Analyst Forecast Errors over the Sample Period Define an analyst's average annual forecast error as: N\" k=l (Fi-Y1;)/P$-', (1) where NIt is the number of earnings forecasts made by analyst i during hth earnings year t for any of the firms in the sample, Fi is analyst i7s forecast of year t annual primary earnings per share for a firm, Yf is actual annual earnings corresponding to analyst i's hth earnings forecast during year t, and Pf-I is the year-end closing price in year t -1 corresponding to the kth forecast of analyst i. Nitcan include multiple forecasts from analyst i for a given firm and year. Forecast error (1) measures an individual analyst's average annual over- or underestimate of actual earnings. Dividing by share price yields forecasted and actual earningslprice ratios. Using beginning-of-year prices reduces the inter- action between year t forecast errors in the numerator and price changes, and hence price, in the denominator. Over long periods, there is usually little evidence of consistent forecast bias (e.g., Brown, Foster, and Noreen [1985]); but over our four-year sample period, at least 69% of individual analysts7 average annual fore- casts fell above average annual earnings (table 1). The mean forecast error divided by share price was .0161 while the mean level of earnings was .0593, when divided by 1986 year-end price. 3. Persistence of Analyst Forecast Performance over Time We examine forecast accuracy and optimism/pessimism by comparing analysts7 forecasts to median consensus forecasts. (Conclusions are the same based on comparisons with a mean consensus.) 07Brien [I9881 152 JOURNAL OF ACCOUNTING RESEARCH, SPRING 1991 TABLE 1 Average Annual Forecast Error Distributions* 1983 1984 1985 -- 1986 Average Entire Sample of Firms: 186 Analysts Mean 0.0061 49Firms StandardDeviations 0.0141 69.9% 9,120 Forecasts Overestimates Underestimates 30.1% 0.0131 0.0237 0.0214 0.0161 0.0244 0.0292 0.0243 0.0230 73.1% 83.9% 79.0% 76.5% 26.9% 16.1% 21.0% 23.5% Firms with Low Value Line Earnings Predictability Rank (0-50): 0.0136 0.0216 0.0388 0.0153 0.0223 70 Analysts Mean 18 Firms Standard Deviations 0.0223 0.0332 0.0481 0.0294 0.0333 71.4% 84.3% 82.9% 60.0% 74.6% 2,232 Forecasts Overestimates Underestimates 28.6% 15.7% 17.1% 40.0% 25.4% Firms with High Value Line Earnings Predictability Rank (51-100): 0.0027 0.0061 0.0149 0.0266 0.0126 134 Analysts Mean Standard Deviations 0.0072 0.0195 0.0172 0.0227 0.0166 31 Firms 5,931 Forecasts Overestimates 61.9% 65.7% 81.3% 91.0% 75.0% Underestimates 38.1% 34.3% 18.7% 9.0% 25.0% * Average annual forecast error is calculated according to equation (1)for each analyst in each sample. Means and standard deviations are calculated across the individual analysts' average annual forecast errors. Overestimates (underestimates) represent the proportion of analysts with average annual fore- casts falling above (below) average annual earnings. finds that the most current forecast is at least as accurate as the mean or median of all available forecasts but that aggregating across current forecasts can further improve accuracy. Given O'Brien's results, we construct median consensus forecasts from at least three competing analysts (i.e., not including the analyst being examined) with forecast dates in the same month. Median estimates exhibit slightly greater forecast accuracy in our sample. Define average annual relative forecast accuracy as: wt h= l 2 [IF: -Y;;f(-ICi -Y:(]/Pii-l, (2) where C;i is the median consensus forecast corresponding to Fii. This measure controls for the average predictability of both the firm and the time of forecast by subtracting the absolute magnitude of consensus forecast errors. Superior analysts are required to \"beat the consensus\" regardless of its accuracy. This approach differs in detail but not in spirit from that of O'Brien [1990]. Ideally, tests of forecast accuracy would control for higher moments of earnings predictability. For instance, analysts of firms with volatile earnings are likely to deviate from both the consensus estimate and actual earnings by a wider margin than those following less volatile firms. Comparing analyst forecast accuracy across disparate earnings realiza- tions disproportionately weights the forecast accuracy of analysts follow- FORECAST ACCURACY OF INDIVIDUAL ANALYSTS 153 ing firms with hard to predict earnings. O'Brien's [1990] emphasis on industry samples controls for this effect. To control for differences in earnings predictability, we dichotomize the sample according to Value Line's earnings predictability ranking (0-50 = low and 51-100 = high predictability). This rank is related to the dispersion of analysts' beliefs. In table 1,the standard deviation of individual analysts' average annual forecast error is higher for analysts following firms of low earnings predictability than for analysts following firms of high earnings predictability. We run our statistical tests on the entire sample as well as on each earnings predictability class. Define average annual relative forecast error as: This measures the magnitude and direction of an analyst's average annual disagreement with the consensus. Optimists consistently forecast above the consensus, while pessimists consistently forecast below. Analysts are ranked in descending order according to (2) and (3) in each sample year. Consistently inferior or optimistic analysts should appear near the top of each year's rankings. Conversely, consistently superior or pessimistic analysts should appear near the bottom of each year's rankings. Table 2 provides the number of analysts consistently falling above (below) each of the top (bottom) five deciles. Dichotomize analyst forecast performance into success Stif an analyst ranks in the top D analysts from a sample of N analysts in year t and failure F, otherwise. Then P[St] = DIN and P[F,] = 1 -DIN. Under the assumption that analyst forecast rankings are independent over time, the joint probability that a given analyst will rank in the top D analysts = Note that once an analyst's rank in each year is P[S1S2S3S4] is determined in a year, no other analyst may possess the same rank except in the case of a tie; therefore, there is dependence of analyst ranks within a single year. We use a simulation approach to estimate the probability distribution. in a given sample. Let X be the number of analysts satisfying S1S2S3S4We estimate the probability mass P[X= c] on each point X = c under the assumption that analyst forecast ranks are independent over time based on 5,000 trials as follows: (1) Draw D observations without replacement from the N ranks. (2) Independently repeat this random sampling for each of the four sample years. (3) Count the number of analysts c that appear among the top D analysts in all four years. Increment the probability mass assigned to point X = c by 1/5000. (4) Repeat the above steps 5,000 times. 154 K. C. BUTLER AND L. H. P. LANG TABLE 2 Number and Significance of Analysts Falling Above (Below) the Kth Decile in Each Year* Decile 1 2 3 4 5 Entire Sample of Firms: Expected ............................. Optimistic ........................... Pessimistic ........................... Inferior ............................. Superior Firms with Low Value Line Earnings Predictability Rank: 0.11 Expected. ............................0.01 Optimistic ...........................0 2 (1.00) (0.00) Pessimistic ......................... 1 1 (0.01) (0.11) Inferior ..............................0 0 (1.00) (1.00) Superior ...........................0 0 (1.00) (1.00) Firms with High Value Line Earnings Predictability Rank: 0.21 Expected. .............................0.01 Optimistic ..........................0 0 (1.00) (1.00) 3 Pessimistic............................1 (0.01) (0.00) 1 Inferior ..............................0 (1.00) (0.18) Superior ............................0 1 (1.00) (0.18) 1.09 2 (0.30) 7 (0.00) 3 (0.08) 2 (0.30) 3.43 5 (0.22) 8 (0.01) 5 (0.22) 6 (0.10) 8.38 14 (0.02) 18 (0.00) 10 (0.31) 12 (0.10) *The number of analysts ranking in the top (optimistic or inferior analysts) or bottom (pessimistic or superior analysts) k deciles (k=1, ...,5) in each year of the sample period. Estimated probability values under the independence assumption from the Monte Carlo simulation appear in parentheses. The only values of c with nonzero probability mass are 0 5 c 5resulting probability distribution provides a one-tailed test of: D.The P[Xm c] =2 P[X=i] =1-2 P[X=i]. I=C D c-1 i=O Probability values generated by this simulation stabilized at the 0.01 level after 4,000 iterations for every point X =c in each of the 15 (3 samples x5 decile thresholds) distributions estimated with the simula- tion procedure. Probability values from this approximation procedure appear in table 2 for the top five deciles in each sample. Note that the fifth-decile probability values are more reliable than the probability values for deciles FORECAST ACCURACY OF INDIVIDUAL ANALYSTS 155 1through 4 because of the larger expected values. For the sample of 186 analysts, a difference of a single analyst at the first-decile level swings the probability value from 1.00, as in the case of inferior and superior analysts, to 0.02, as for optimistic and pessimistic analysts. While we report probability values for deciles 1through 4, we base our conclusions solely on the fifth-decile results. We find no statistically significant evidence (a = .05) of persistent differential forecast accuracy at the fifth-decile threshold. In contrast, statistically significant evidence of persistent optimism and pessimism exists at the fifth-decile threshold in the entire sample and in both earnings predictability classes. A majority of observed values in the first four deciles are also greater than the expected number. Our sample selection criteria result in a highly selected sample of long- surviving and relatively active analysts following large firms. The finding of persistent analyst optimism and pessimism is especially surprising in this sample. The earnings forecast behavior of less active analysts or of analysts following smaller firms may be different. Pessimistic analysts should enjoy superior forecast accuracy because of the consistently positive observed forecast errors over our sample period. To examine the relation between analyst optimism/pessimism and relative forecast accuracy, we constructed 3 x 3 contingency tables of analysts' average rankings over all four years according to (2) and (3). Chi-square statistics based on these tables indicate that analysts' average optimism/pessimism is associated with lowerlhigher average forecast accuracy (significant at the .03 level) in the entire sample and in both earnings predictability classes. Several characteristics of analysts' forecasts could influence the finding of persistent analyst forecast behavior. Just as analysts forecasting earnings for firms of low earnings predictability may have more variable forecast errors, analysts providing fewer forecasts per year or reporting forecasts earlier in the year may have more variable average annual forecast errors. Analysts' average number of forecasts per year and average time of forecast during the year (January = 1and December = 12) were compared to the four-year average relative forecast accuracy (2) and relative forecast error (3)measures. Based on correlations and cross- tabulations, these analyst characteristics are statistically unrelated to relative forecast error and relative forecast accuracy. 4. Conclusion Analysts in our sample persist over time in their optimism or pessimism relative to consensus earnings forecasts, for firms of both high and low earnings predictability. Individual analysts show little evidence of per- sistent differential forecast ability. We conclude that observed differences in forecast accuracy in our sample depend on the interaction of analyst optimism or pessimism with observed forecast errors and not on differ- ential forecasting ability on the part of individual analysts. 156 K. C. BUTLER AND L. H. P. LANG REFERENCES BROWN,P.; G. FOSTER;AND E. NOREEN. Security Analyst Multi-Year Earnings Forecasts and the Capital Market. American Accounting Association Studies in Accounting Re- search, no. 21. Sarasota, Fla.: American Accounting Assn., 1985. O'BRIEN,P. \"Analysts' Forecasts as Earnings Expectations.\" Journal of Accounting and Economics (1988): 53-83. -.\"Forecast Accuracy of Individual Analysts in Nine Industries.\" Journal of Account- ing Research (Autumn 1990): 286-304. http://www.jstor.org
LINKEDCITATIONS
-Page1of1-
Youhaveprintedthefollowingarticle:
TheForecastAccuracyofIndividualAnalysts:EvidenceofSystematicOptimismandPessimism
KirtC.Butler;LarryH.P.Lang
JournalofAccountingResearch,Vol.29,No.1.(Spring,1991),pp.150-156.
StableURL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0021-8456%28199121%2929%3A1%3C150%3ATFAOIA%3E2.0.CO%3B2-RThisarticlereferencesthefollowinglinkedcitations.Ifyouaretryingtoaccessarticlesfroman
off-campuslocation,youmayberequiredtofirstlogonviayourlibrarywebsitetoaccessJSTOR.Pleasevisityourlibrary'swebsiteorcontactalibrariantolearnaboutoptionsforremoteaccesstoJSTOR.
References
ForecastAccuracyofIndividualAnalystsinNineIndustriesPatriciaC.O'Brien
JournalofAccountingResearch,Vol.28,No.2.(Autumn,1990),pp.286-304.
StableURL:
http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0021-8456%28199023%2928%3A2%3C286%3AFAOIAI%3E2.0.CO%3B2-P
因篇幅问题不能全部显示,请点此查看更多更全内容